In What Ways Do the Performing Arts Reflect Emotions?

Introduction

Emotional responses to art (i.e., aesthetic emotions) have long interested philosophers, psychologists, and art critics (Robinson, 2004). Theories in psychology and aesthetics (James, 1890/1950; Bell, 1914; Berlyne, 1974) initially focused on positive emotional responses that arise from the appreciation of the form of expression as beautiful, harmonious, or powerful (Robinson, 2004). Recent studies have found that indeed, emotions (i.due east., brief affective states triggered by the appraisal of an event in relation to current goals; Scherer and Zentner, 2001) such as awe (Shiota et al., 2007) and wonder (Zentner et al., 2008) are oft reported in relation to the contemplation of artworks. These emotions typically occur when an object or result is appraised as highly complex and novel, and creates a sense of being in the presence of something greater than oneself (Keltner and Haidt, 2003).

Notwithstanding, it has likewise been recently emphasized that affective responses to art are more various (Silvia, 2011) and often include emotions such as sadness (Vuoskoski and Eerola, 2012) and nostalgia (Barrett et al., 2010), which are as well experienced in other everyday situations that do not involve contemplation of artworks. These emotions may be related to the content and personal interpretation of an artwork, rather than its course (Robinson, 2004; Silvia, 2011). For instance, i may admire Caravaggio's skill in David with the Head of Goliath, but also experience cloy at the sight of dripping blood, and sadness at the idea that this artwork may express the painter'due south remorse. Similarly, someone listening to the Adagietto from Mahler'south 5th Symphony may experience blends of awe, tenderness and nostalgia related to the expert orchestration, on the one hand, and knowing that this slice captures the composer'southward dear for his wife and worries for his deteriorating health, on the other hand. Therefore, art contemplation can trigger multiple emotions, which include aesthetic emotions driven by positive appraisals of the form of expression, and other positive or negative emotions, driven past appraisals of the content or pregnant of artworks (Silvia, 2011). Given the increasing involvement in affective science (Gross and Barrett, 2013), recent studies have focused on describing emotions associated with aesthetic experiences such as looking at painting and listening to music, and on examining their underlying mechanisms and motivation (for review see Silvia, 2011; Swaminathan and Schellenberg, 2015).

Influential theoretical frameworks, which have guided inquiry on preferences for painting (Leder et al., 2004; Lindell and Mueller, 2011) and emotional responses to music (Scherer and Zentner, 2001), argue that i's reactions to artworks involve an interplay of multiple factors related to stimulus, person, and situation. The contribution of perceptual features and formal characteristics conveying style has been pointed out by observations that aesthetic preferences form very rapidly (i.e., in less than i s), whether in the class of beauty judgments of graphic patterns (Jacobsen and Höfel, 2003) or emotional categorization of music excerpts (Bigand et al., 2005). Indeed, these rapid responses may involve automated mechanisms such equally visual disambiguation (Topolinski et al., 2015) and premotor simulation (Leder et al., 2012; Ticini et al., 2014), although recent studies also study their interaction with consciously controlled processes such equally expectations (McLean et al., 2015). The relations between the structural characteristics of music (e.k., mode, tempo) and emotional responses take been systematically investigated (Gabrielsson and Lindstrom, 2001; Gomez and Danuser, 2007). Taking a more full general approach, enquiry relevant to painting has mostly focused on non-aesthetic stimuli (e.g., geometrical shapes) and broad aesthetic preferences instead of specific emotions (Jacobsen and Höfel, 2002). Notwithstanding, theory in both fields (Scherer and Zentner, 2001; Lindell and Mueller, 2011) has acknowledged that stimulus-driven or "bottom-upwardly" processing interacts with didactics and psychological characteristics that can influence emotional responses to art through noesis-driven or "pinnacle-down" processing.

Many studies have therefore examined whether art pedagogy facilitates fine art-related emotions through a improve understanding of the formal ways of expression in painting or music. Indeed, students in art history compared to students in other fields categorize paintings using more criteria and favor style-related rather than affective criteria (Augustin and Leder, 2006). Similarly, musicians perceive the links betwixt a musical theme and its variations better than not-musicians (Bigand and Poulin-Charronnat, 2006), and describe music using adjectives related to novelty and originality rather than emotional characteristics (Istok et al., 2009). However, despite these differences in processing styles, music-related emotions are not markedly unlike in musicians and non-musicians (Bigand and Poulin-Charronnat, 2006; Baltes et al., 2012) and the same may be true for painting-related emotions. While no study investigated the influence of visual arts expertise on emotional responses to painting, experimental show suggests that providing boosted data that facilitates agreement of paintings does non influence preference for paintings (Leder et al., 2006). In addition to fine art educational activity, other individual differences such as prior mood may too influence emotional responses to artworks (Hunter et al., 2011; Vuoskoski and Eerola, 2011a,b; Baltes and Miu, 2014).

Situational factors may besides attune fine art-related emotions. For instance, the presence of other people such as in the attendance of live music operation or during a visit to an art gallery may influence emotional responses to artworks. Field studies (Juslin et al., 2008) and experimental studies (Liljestrom et al., 2013) showed that the presence of the romantic partner or a close friend during music listening increases the frequency of melancholia states such as happiness-elation, pleasance-enjoyment and admiration-awe. These findings highlight social facilitation equally one of the factors that may contribute to the increased enjoyment of music during live music functioning (Lamont, 2011). The influence of context has besides been acknowledged in theories of painting-related emotions (Leder et al., 2004) and one study (Pelowski et al., 2014) suggested that social encounters in art galleries may be detrimental to aesthetic experience past inducing competition between social awareness and self-focused enjoyment of paintings. Even so, the influence of social factors and other contextual variables (eastward.g., location; Scherer and Zentner, 2001) needs farther enquiry, peculiarly in the example of painting-related emotions.

In addition to mechanisms, recent studies have too focused on motivation for exposure to art. The almost commonly reported reason for music listening is "mood repair" or emotion regulation, just social reasons (e.g., convalesce loneliness; keep upwards with art trends) and self-actualization needs (east.g., explore and express identity) are also frequently reported (Lonsdale and Northward, 2011). People use music to manage their mood to a greater extent than they use other leisure activities such as reading or exercising (Lonsdale and North, 2011). However, the tendency to use music for mood repair may be influenced past music training considering that musicians utilize music for cognitive (due east.k., attention to structural complexity or performing technique) rather than emotional reasons (Getz et al., 2014). To our noesis, no written report has however investigated motivation for aesthetic experience with painting.

In summary, painting and music-related emotions seem to involve a like interplay of factors related to stimulus, person and context. However, whatever try to generalize beyond experience with these arts is currently hampered by the lack of empirical evidence on certain issues, specially in the case of painting (due east.g., frequency of specific emotions; influence of visual arts expertise, prior mood and social context; motivation), as well as the absence of integrative studies systematically comparing the characteristics of aesthetic experience in relation to painting and music (but see Rawlings et al., 2000; Cleridou and Furnham, 2014). In this report, parallel surveys on the feel of looking at painting and listening to music were filled in online by two samples of volunteers. Self-reported frequency of emotions, evaluation of contributing factors, and motivation in artful experience with painting and music were compared between samples. In addition, the influence of art didactics on the characteristics of artful experience was also investigated.

Material and Methods

Participants

The surveys on looking at painting and listening to music were separately advertised online, mainly through social media (e.chiliad., Facebook), as role of a psychological study on aesthetic experience. The survey on looking at painting was filled in past 260 participants, and the survey on listening to music was filled in by 711 participants. The surveys were in Romanian and all participants reported Romanian as their first language. Table 1 shows the distributions of age, sex activity, general education, and occupational status, which were not significantly different between the samples. Participants were informed that they would answer questions about their feel of looking at painting or listening to music, and signed a consent form before accessing the survey. The study followed the recommendations of the Declaration of Helsinki regarding participant safety and was approved by the Ideals Commission of Babeş-Bolyai University.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics of the survey samples.

Surveys

The questions and answer options were equivalent in the two surveys. Other than the reference to painting or music, the phrasing was identical.

The surveys were divided into three sections. The first section focused on socio-demographic characteristics: age, sex, didactics level, and occupational status.

The second section surveyed art education, asking participants whether they had graduated from a high schoolhouse or college in the field of visual arts or music. Participants who filled in the survey on painting-related experiences were also asked to report whether they had knowledge related to painting or drawing, sculpture, and/or fine art history. Those who filled in the survey on music-related experiences were asked to study whether they had knowledge related to sight reading of musical scores, musical instrument playing and/or musicology. They were as well asked to assess how experienced they thought they were in looking at painting or listening to music (five-bespeak calibration: ane, beginner; 5, experienced), as well as the personal importance of these art-related activities (5-point scale: 1, not at all important; five, very important).

The third section included questions about frequency of art-related emotions, perception of contributing factors, and motivation for aesthetic experience. Emotional experience was assessed by request participants to rate the frequency of several emotions in relation to looking at painting or listening to music, using a five-betoken scale (1, never; 2, rarely; iii, sometimes; 4, oft; 5, very oftentimes). The emotion labels were taken from the 25-item version of the Geneva Emotional Music Calibration (Zentner et al., 2008), representing nine emotion categories: wonder, transcendence, tenderness, nostalgia and peacefulness (facets of the more full general dimension of "sublimity"); power and blithesome activation (facets of "vitality"); and tension and sadness (facets of "unease"). To our knowledge, GEMS is the merely standardized musical instrument covering the whole spectrum of emotional responses to artworks, including both positive aesthetic emotions (e.m., wonder, transcendence), and other positive (e.1000., blithesome activation, power) and negative emotions (eastward.yard., nostalgia, sadness) that occur in various situations in everyday life. At that place is no equivalent standardized cess of emotional responses to painting and developing such an instrument was beyond the purpose of this written report. Notwithstanding, we thought GEMS was suitable for this exploratory study considering the potential similarities between emotional responses to music (Zentner and Eerola, 2010) and painting (Silvia, 2011). The Romanian translation of GEMS was used in several previous studies (e.thousand., Miu and Baltes, 2012; Baltes and Miu, 2014).

In addition to assessing the frequency of emotions using GEMS, another detail asked participants to charge per unit the similarity between everyday emotions and emotional feel with painting or music using a five-point scale (1, not at all; 5, very much).

Participants likewise rated, on a scale from i (not at all) to 5 (very much), the extent to which painting or music-related emotions involved one of the following factors: (ane) structural features of the aesthetic stimulus, such as grade, colour, contrasts and composition for painting, and manner and tempo for music; (2) concrete context (e.g., location); (3) prior mood, immediately earlier exposure to artworks; (4) previous cognition nigh artwork and artist (i.e., painter or composer); and (v) presence of other people, when aesthetic experience occurs in social contexts. These factors were inspired past previous studies (Scherer and Zentner, 2001).

Some other item focused on motivation, and participants were asked to rate the importance of 5 potential reasons in their aesthetic experience with painting or music: (one) mood management or relaxation; (2) experiencing new emotions, which are non typical of everyday life; (3) self-didactics; (four) sharing emotions with others; and (five) keeping company when one feels lone. These types of motivation were also derived from previous literature (Lonsdale and North, 2011).

Statistical Analyses

The main analyses compared cocky-reported frequency of emotions, contributing factors and motivation for the 2 types of aesthetic experience: looking at painting and listening to music. Other analyses compared betwixt participants with and without fine art education. Considering the unequal sizes of the two samples, as well as of the groups with and without art education, nosotros used analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Welch's correction for unequal variance, which is a robust method to protect against type I errors while conserving power (Kohr and Games, 1974). In add-on, we used the Bonferroni method to correct the threshold of statistical significance for each set of analyses, as follows: p ≤ 0.005 (0.05/9) for self-reported frequency of emotions; p ≤ 0.01 (0.05/5) for perceived contributing factors; and p ≤ 0.01 (0.05/5) for self-reported motivation. Event sizes are reported equally η P ii , where an outcome of 0.01 is small-scale, one of 0.06 is medium, and one of 0.fourteen is large (Cohen, 1988). All analyses were run in SPSS.

Results

Painting and Music-Related Emotions

By comparing self-reported frequency of each emotion between samples (Effigy one), we establish that those who described their feel of looking at painting reported higher frequencies of wonder compared to those who described their feel of listening to music [F (1, 525.29) = 28.49, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.03]. In contrast, the frequencies of tenderness [F (1, 434.56) = 33.86, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.04], nostalgia [F (one, 419.57) = 30.09, p < 0.001, η P two = 0.03], peacefulness [F (ane, 438.95) = 35.83, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.04], power [F (ane, 447.32) = 89.75, p < 0.001, η P ii = 0.09], joyful activation [F (1, 410.84) = 151.69, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.15], and sadness [F (1, 501.01) = 43.55, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.04] were higher in relation to listening to music compared to looking at painting. The frequency of transcendence and tension were not different in the ii samples.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure i. Perceived frequency of emotions in the experience of looking at painting and listening to music. Error bars point standard mistake of the mean. **p < 0.01.

The perceived similarity between art-related emotions and everyday emotions was as well analyzed. Painting-related emotions (One thousand = 3.25; SD = 0.99) were rated as significantly less similar to emotions in other everyday situations, compared to music-related emotions (M = 3.53; SD = 0.97): F (ane, 450.29) = 15.89, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.02.

Perception of Contributing Factors

Figure ii shows the perceived contributions of several factors to art-related emotions. The contributions of stimulus features [F (1, 624.81) = 56.85, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.04] and previous knowledge [F (ane, 461.09) = 12.48, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.01] were rated at higher levels for painting-related emotions, whereas the contributions of prior mood [F (1, 384.lx) = 65.93, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.08], physical context [F (ane, 437.99) = xxx.29, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.03], and the presence of others [F (1, 433.12) = 44.99, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.05] were rated at higher levels for music-related emotions.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Perception of factors contributing to painting and music-related emotions. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. **p < 0.01.

Self-Reported Motivation

Self-reported motivation was besides compared between participants who described their experience of looking at painting and listening to music (Effigy three). Cocky-educational activity was rated equally more than important for looking at painting [F (1, 481.05) = 48.48, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.05], whereas mood management [F (1, 375.83) = 125.61, p < 0.001, η P two = 0.14] and keeping company [F (ane, 506.15) = fifty.21, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.05] were rated as more of import for music listening. Experiencing new emotions and sharing emotions with others were rated at comparable levels for looking at painting and music listening.

www.frontiersin.org

Effigy 3. Self-reported motivation for looking at painting and listening to music. Error bars indicate standard error of the hateful. **p < 0.01.

Fine art Teaching

There were 69 visual arts graduates in the sample that answered the painting survey, and 42 music graduates in the sample that answered the music survey. The majority of visual arts graduates reported knowledge about painting (99.65%), sculpture (55.07%), and art history (95.65%). The self-reported level of experience with painting [t (258) = 7.53, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = ane.05), and the personal importance of painting [t (258) = 5.04, p < 0.001, Cohen'south d = 0.72] were significantly higher for visual fine art graduates compared to the other participants who filled in the painting survey. Similarly, most music graduates reported knowledge related to sight reading of music scores (92.86%), musical instrument playing (95.23%), and musicology (85.71%). The self-reported levels of feel with music [t (51.74) = 7.56, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 1.02] and the personal importance of music [t (64.99) = 5.95, p < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.65] were significantly higher for music graduates compared to the other participants who filled in the survey on listening to music.

Next, self-reported frequency of emotions, perception of contributing factors, and self-reported motivation for looking at painting and listening to music were compared between participants with and without fine art instruction in each sample (Table 2).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Perceived frequency of emotions in participants with and without arts teaching.

Participants with visual arts educational activity reported significantly higher frequencies of power [F (one, 106.04) = 10.18, p = 0.002, η P ii = 0.04] and joyful activation [F (i, 120.54) = 17.32, p < 0.001, η P 2 = 0.06] in their experience with painting, in comparison to participants without visual arts education. Frequencies of the other painting-related emotions were not significantly different betwixt those with and without visual arts education. Self-reported frequencies of all music-related emotions were similar in participants with and without music education.

Perceived similarity between art-related (i.due east., painting or music) and everyday emotions was not significantly unlike in participants with and without art education (i.e., visual arts education or music education).

Both participants with visual arts instruction [F (1, 122.63) = vi.81, p = 0.010, η P ii = 0.03] and those with music teaching [F (1, 46.04) = 23.91, p < 0.001, η P ii = 0.03] rated the contribution of previous knowledge to painting-related emotions and music-related emotions, respectively, as more than important, in comparison to participants without art teaching (Table 3).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Perception of factors contributing to art-related emotions in participants with and without arts educational activity.

There were no significant differences related to art education in self-reported motivation for looking at painting or listening to music (Table 4).

www.frontiersin.org

Table four. Self-reported motivation for looking at painting and listening to music in participants with and without arts teaching.

Discussion

In this study, participants answered surveys on their experience of looking at painting and listening to music. The master aims were to compare between perceptions regarding frequency of emotions, contribution of several factors to fine art-related emotions, and motivation for these two types of aesthetic experience. In addition, nosotros examined the influence of fine art educational activity on these dimensions.

Previous studies identified emotions that are commonly experienced past music listeners (Zentner et al., 2008). Aesthetic emotions such as awe (Shiota et al., 2007) and other positive and negative emotions that occur in various everyday situations (Silvia, 2011) have too been described in the experience of looking at painting. These studies suggested that looking at painting and listening to music are associated with blends of different types of emotions. However, no report has still compared the relative frequency of dissimilar emotions in these ii types of aesthetic experience. The present results indicate that wonder may be more frequently experienced while looking at painting rather than while listening to music. In add-on, the experience of looking at painting may be associated with relatively lower frequency of vitality-related emotions (Zentner et al., 2008) such as joyful activation and power. These two emotions were much more frequently (i.e., large or medium effect size) reported in relation to listening to music, which suggests that "vitality" may all-time distinguish emotional responses to music and painting. Other emotions (i.east., tenderness, nostalgia, peacefulness, sadness) were as well more than frequently reported in the feel of listening to music compared to looking at painting, but to a lesser degree, that is, with minor effect sizes.

Painting-related emotions were perceived every bit less like to emotions experienced in other everyday life situations compared to music-related emotions. This perception may be connected to the relatively higher frequency of wonder associated with looking at painting, considering that this emotion is experienced in limited contexts (eastward.thou., contemplation of artworks or nature scenes; Shiota et al., 2007) that create the awareness of beingness in the presence of something greater than oneself (Keltner and Haidt, 2003). The reduced vitality of emotions associated with looking at painting may also contribute to the impression that they are unlike from emotional experience in general.

These results also signal differences in the perception of factors that may contribute to art-related emotions. Participants rated stimulus features and previous noesis equally making more important contributions to emotional responses to painting than to music. These impressions are in line with theories (Berlyne, 1974) and experimental show (Jacobsen and Höfel, 2002; Leder et al., 2012; Ticini et al., 2014; McLean et al., 2015; Topolinski et al., 2015) that support the relation between perceptual features of paintings and their emotional touch on. The present observations practice not exclude the contribution of these factors to music-induced emotions, which is well documented in the literature (Gabrielsson and Lindstrom, 2001; Gomez and Danuser, 2007), only merely suggest that people perceive them every bit weighing more than in the experience of looking at painting. In addition, the perception that previous knowledge plays an important office in painting-related emotions was corroborated by another observation in this report (see beneath), namely that the frequency of certain painting-related emotions was college in visual art graduates, who reported higher levels of fine art knowledge. In a complementary way, the influence of prior mood, concrete context, and the presence of other people were rated equally more important in relation to music-induced emotions. These subjective evaluations are likewise in line with previous testify showing that indeed, both mood prior to music exposure, whether in laboratory (Hunter et al., 2011; Vuoskoski and Eerola, 2011b) or concert hall (Vuoskoski and Eerola, 2011b; Baltes and Miu, 2014), and the presence of others, specially close persons (Juslin et al., 2008; Liljestrom et al., 2013), influence emotional responses to music.

Experiences of looking at painting and listening to music were too differentiated past self-reported motivation. Relatively more participants reported that cocky-education motivated them to wait at painting. In addition, relatively more participants reported that mood repair and keeping company drove their feel of listening to music. These motivational differences may be supported by many factors, including the wider accessibility of music on portable devices, which may increase its use for everyday life needs such as mood repair (Lonsdale and Due north, 2011), and the relatively higher vitality of emotional responses to music, which may contribute to increasing role in everyday life. Pending on replication of these results, time to come enquiry could examine why people use the experience of looking at painting and listening to music for relatively different reasons.

Visual arts graduates reported higher frequencies of power and joyful activation in their feel of looking at painting. Considering that these emotions had the everyman frequencies in the overall sample that answered the painting survey, this indicates that visual arts formal training has a significant impact on emotional responses to painting and may specifically enhance vitality-related emotions. In contrast, music formal training had no pregnant result on the frequency of music-related emotions, which is in line with previous prove (Bigand and Poulin-Charronnat, 2006; Baltes et al., 2012). These findings suggest that painting-related emotions may involve cognition-driven or top-down information processing to a larger extent than music-related emotions. However, both visual arts and music graduates rated the contribution of previous cognition (e.m., data about artwork and artist) to emotional responses at college levels than participants without formal art training. No differences in motivation for looking at painting and listening to music were linked to formal art instruction. Given that art graduates reported increased levels of art-related cognition—although note that this type of knowledge was non express to those with formal preparation—, as well as increased experience with and personal importance of fine art, these differences may have driven the present observations on the influence of formal art training.

This study has at least ii main limitations. First, being based on surveys, these findings depict how fine art-related feel is perceived past people, and may thus be subjectively biased. For case, all fine art graduates reported that increased levels of art noesis would enhance fine art-related emotions, just only visual arts instruction seemed to influence emotional responses to painting. Second, we assessed emotional experience using a calibration that focuses on emotions which are mutual in the experience of listening to music. There is no similar scale for painting-related emotions, so the only available options for this study were measures focused on music-induced emotions such as GEMS (Shiota et al., 2007) and general measures such as PANAS (Watson and Clark, 1994). We chose the sometime pick because that GEMS, which was developed through a factorial approach based on self-reported feel of music listeners (Zentner et al., 2008), may offering a more than specific assessment of aesthetic emotions, leaving out emotions that are not representative for the feel of music listening and may be equally unrepresentative for the experience of looking at painting. Previous studies suggested some similarities between emotional responses to painting and music (Shiota et al., 2007). In addition, GEMS and PANAS partially overlap, with emotions like wonder, power, blithesome activation, tension, and sadness from the old scale paralleling emotions like serenity, self-balls, joviality, hostility, and sadness from the latter scale. Nevertheless these reasons in favor of our approach, information technology is possible that we did not appraise emotions that are more than specific to looking at painting and are not covered by GEMS. For instance, recent studies identified and then-called "cognition emotions" such as surprise, involvement and confusion in the experience of looking at painting (Silvia, 2011). Therefore, the specificity of painting-related emotions may take been underestimated in this study. Future research may place other specific aspects of emotional responses to painting.

In conclusion, our results highlighted multiple differences in the perceived qualities of looking at painting and listening to music: emotional responses to painting may exist characterized by higher levels of wonder and lower vitality, and are perceived as less like to emotions in other everyday life situations, compared to music-induced emotions; people outweigh the contributions of stimulus features and previous knowledge in relation to emotional responses to painting, and the contributions of prior mood, concrete context, and the presence of others in relation to emotional responses to music; looking at painting is driven by self-didactics motivation, whereas listening to music is associated with emotional and social motivation; and formal fine art training influences emotional responses to painting (e.g., by increasing vitality), merely not to music, which suggests that the quondam may depend more on knowledge-driven information processing. We hope this study will encourage the integration of theories and approaches in research on painting and music, which accept largely adult in parallel until now, and stimulate future inquiry that could give a more detailed perspective on common and specific aspects of aesthetic experiences with unlike forms of fine art.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absenteeism of any commercial or fiscal relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the Editor and the two reviewers for helping us to amend this manuscript. We are specially grateful to Reviewer ane for detailed and constructive comments.

References

Augustin, M. D., and Leder, H. (2006). Fine art expertise: a written report of concepts and conceptual spaces. Psychol. Sci. 48, 135–156.

Google Scholar

Baltes, F. R., Miclea, M., and Miu, A. C. (2012). Does everybody like Vivaldi'south Four Seasons? Affective space and a comparison of music-induced emotions betwixt musicians and non-musicians. Cogn. Brain Behav. Interdiscip. J. 16, 107–119.

Google Scholar

Baltes, F. R., and Miu, A. C. (2014). Emotions during live music performance: links with private differences in empathy, visual imagery, and mood. Psychomusicol. Music Mind Encephalon 24, 58–65. doi: 10.1037/pmu0000030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, F. S., Grimm, Grand. J., Robins, R. W., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., and Janata, P. (2010). Music-evoked nostalgia: affect, memory, and personality. Emotion 10, 390–403. doi: 10.1037/a0019006

PubMed Abstruse | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berlyne, D. E. (1974). Studies in the New Experimental Aesthetics. New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Bigand, E., and Poulin-Charronnat, B. (2006). Are we "experienced listeners"? A review of the musical capacities that practise not depend on formal musical grooming. Knowledge 100, 100–130. doi: 10.1016/j.knowledge.2005.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleridou, K., and Furnham, A. (2014). Personality correlates of artful preferences for fine art, architecture, and music. Empir. Stud. Arts 32, 231–255. doi: x.2190/EM.32.two.f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Ability Assay for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Gabrielsson, A., and Lindstrom, Eastward. (2001). "The influence of musical structure on emotional expression," in Music and Emotion: Theory and Research, eds P. Northward. Juslin and J. A. Sloboda (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 223–248.

Google Scholar

Getz, L. M., Marks, S., and Roy, Chiliad. (2014). The influence of stress, optimism, and music training on music uses and preferences. Psychol. Music 42, 71–85. doi: ten.1177/0305735612456727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Istok, East., Brattico, Due east., Jacobsen, T., Krohn, K., Muller, M., and Tervaniemi, G. (2009). Aesthetic responses to music: a questionnaire study. Musicae Sci. 13, 183–206. doi: 10.1177/102986490901300201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobsen, T., and Höfel, L. (2002). Aesthetic judgments of novel graphic patterns: analyses of individual judgments. Percept. Mot. Skills 95, 755–766. doi: 10.2466/pms.2002.95.3.755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobsen, T., and Höfel, L. (2003). Descriptive and evaluative judgment processes: behavioral and electrophysiological indices of processing symmetry and aesthetics. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 289–299. doi: x.3758/CABN.3.4.289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1890/1950). The Principles of Psychology. New York, NY: Dover.

Juslin, P. Due north., Liljeström, S., Västfjäll, D., Barradas, G., and Silva, A. (2008). An experience sampling study of emotional reactions to music: listener, music, and state of affairs. Emotion eight, 668–683. doi: 10.1037/a0013505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keltner, D., and Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and artful emotion. Cogn. Emot. 17, 297–314. doi: 10.1080/02699930302297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohr, R. L., and Games, P. A. (1974). Robustness of the analysis of variance, the Welch procedure and a box procedure to heterogeneous variances. J. Exp. Educ. 43, 61–69. doi: ten.1080/00220973.1974.10806305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamont, A. (2011). Academy students' potent experiences of music: pleasure, appointment, and meaning. Musicae Sci. 15, 229–249. doi: 10.1177/1029864911403368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leder, H., Carbon, C. C., and Ripsas, A. L. (2006). Entitling art: influence of championship information on understanding and appreciation of paintings. Acta Psychol. 121, 176–198. doi: x.1016/j.actpsy.2005.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liljestrom, S., Juslin, P. N., and Vastfjall, D. (2013). Experimental testify of the roles of music choice, social context, and listener personality in emotional reactions to music. Psychol. Music 41, 579–599. doi: 10.1177/0305735612440615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindell, A. K., and Mueller, J. (2011). Can science business relationship for taste? Psychological insights into fine art appreciation. J. Cogn. Psychol. 23, 453–475. doi: x.1080/20445911.2011.539556

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLean, C., Want, Southward. C., and Dyson, B. J. (2015). The part of similarity, sound and awareness in the appreciation of visual artwork via motor simulation. Knowledge 137, 174–181. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Miu, A. C., and Baltes, F. R. (2012). Empathy manipulation impacts music-induced emotions: a psychophysiological written report on opera. PLoS ONE vii:e30618. doi: ten.1371/journal.pone.0030618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelowski, M., Liu, T., Palacios, V., and Akiba, F. (2014). When a torso meets a body: an exploration of the negative bear upon of social interactions on museum experiences of art. Int. J. Educ. Arts 15, 1–47.

Google Scholar

Rawlings, D., Vidal, N. B. I., and Furnham, A. (2000). Personality and aesthetic preference in Spain and England: 2 studies relating sensation seeking and openness to experience to liking for paintings and music. Eur. J. Pers. 14, 553–576. doi: x.1002/1099-0984(200011/12)xiv:6<553::AID-PER384>3.0.CO;2-H

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, J. (2004). "The emotions in art," in The Blackwell Guide to Aesthetics, ed P. Kivy (Oxford: Blackwell Publishing), 174–192.

Google Scholar

Scherer, Yard. R., and Zentner, M. R. (2001). "Emotional effects of music: product rules," in Music and Emotion: Theory and Research, eds P. N. Juslin and J. A. Sloboda (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 361–392.

Google Scholar

Shiota, Thousand. N., Keltner, D., and Mossman, A. (2007). The nature of awe: elicitors, appraisals, and effects on self-concept. Cogn. Emot. 21, 944–963. doi: x.1080/02699930600923668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvia, P. J. (2011). "Human being emotions and aesthetic experience: an overview of empirical aesthetics," in Aesthetic Science: Connecting Minds, Brains, and Experience, eds A. P. Shimamura and S. East. Palmer (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 250–275.

Swaminathan, S., and Schellenberg, East. G. (2015). Current emotion research in music psychology. Emot. Rev. vii, 189–197. doi: 10.1177/1754073914558282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ticini, 50. F., Rachman, L., Pelletier, J., and Dubal, S. (2014). Enhancing aesthetic appreciation by priming canvases with actions that match the artist's painting manner. Front. Hum. Neurosci. eight:391. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Topolinski, S., Erle, T. Thou., and Reber, R. (2015). Necker'southward grinning: immediate melancholia consequences of early on perceptual processes. Noesis 140, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuoskoski, J. One thousand., and Eerola, T. (2011a). Measuring music-induced emotion: a comparing of emotion models, personality biases, and intensity of experiences. Musicae Sci. 15, 159–173. doi: 10.1177/1029864911403367

CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Vuoskoski, J. Grand., and Eerola, T. (2011b). The office of mood and personality in the perception of emotions represented by music. Cortex 47, 1099–1106. doi: x.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.011

PubMed Abstruse | CrossRef Total Text | Google Scholar

Vuoskoski, J. K., and Eerola, T. (2012). Tin sad music actually brand y'all sad? Indirect measures of affective states induced by music and autobiographical memories. Psychol. Aesthetics Inventiveness Arts 6, 204–213. doi: ten.1037/a0026937

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., and Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-10: Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule - Expanded Course. Iowa: The University of Iowa.

Google Scholar

Zentner, M., and Eerola, T. (2010). "Self-study measures and models of musical emotions," in Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications, eds P. North. Juslin and J. A. Sloboda (New York, NY: Oxford University Printing), 187–223.

Zentner, M., Grandjean, D., and Scherer, K. R. (2008). Emotions evoked by the sound of music: characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion viii, 494–521. doi: ten.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494

PubMed Abstruse | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

deyfaccons.blogspot.com

Source: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01951/full

0 Response to "In What Ways Do the Performing Arts Reflect Emotions?"

Post a Comment

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel